Dog Speak: The Sounds of Dogs · The Wildest

Skip to main content

Dog Speak: The Sounds of Dogs

Decoding the many sounds your pup makes

by Julie Hecht, MSc
31 October 2023
Dog Lays On The Ground Barking
Koen Meershoek / Stocksy

If there’s one universal truth for all dog parents, it’s that we’re all convinced our pups are talking to us. The sigh that means, ‘Stop posting pics of me on Instagram and take me for a walk!’ The grumble that means, ‘It’s only 4.30pm but I’m ready for dinner.’

Vocalisations provide us with a window into dogs’ everyday experiences. Social species are known to be much noisier than animals who lead solitary lives. So given that dogs (and their wild progenitor, the wolf) are super-social, it’s no surprise that they produce a wide range of vocalisations: they bark, whine, whimper, howl, huff, growl, yelp and yip (among other nuanced sounds). From the earliest moments of their lives, dogs produce yelps and whines, and atonal barks and grunts appear in the first few weeks of life in conjunction with the onset of social behaviour.

There’s a big difference between the bark of an adult dog and that of an adult wolf, however. Dogs seem to play every instrument in the orchestra, hitting the highs of Mariah Carey and the lows of Morgan Freeman. Plus, there seems to be no context in which a dog won’t bark: they bark when alone and with other dogs. Some bark before, during and even after a ball is thrown. A car goes by or the doorbell rings and barking ensues. In contrast, wolves bark less frequently and in fewer contexts, primarily for warning or defence.

What do barks mean?

So what do all of those yelps and grunts and yips really mean? Animal behaviour researchers have only recently begun to answer this question. “Vocal behaviour in other species has received a lot of detailed attention,” says Monique Udell, the director of the Human-Animal Interaction Laboratory at the University of Oregon. “In birds, we’ve looked down to the note sequence and explored tiny variations. Vocalisations are such a prominent feature of dogs, and there is a lot to learn.” To date, dog vocalisations have not received comparable scrutiny.

Growls

That being said, research that has been conducted on the subject is incredibly insightful. Take growls, which, it has been shown, dogs use to accurately judge another dog’s size. How in the world do we know that? Tamás Faragó and his colleagues at the Family Dog Project at Eötvös Loránd University in Budapest presented dogs with two images of the same dog: one was true to size and another was 30 percent larger or smaller. Dogs then listened to a pre-recorded growl, and most dogs looked at the image of the full-size dog rather than the altered image.


Growls appear to be meaningful in other ways as well. In another study, Faragó and his colleagues used some clever trickery to explore how dogs respond to growls recorded in different situations. In an apparently empty room, a dog was allowed to approach a bone. Unbeknownst to the dog, there was a speaker hidden behind the bone, and as the dog approached, the sound of a ‘play’ growl, a ‘stranger-approaching’ growl, or a ‘food-guarding’ growl was transmitted through it. Dogs were likely to take the bone when hearing the stranger-approaching or play growl, but the food-guarding ‘my bone’ growl deterred them. Even though the food-guarding and stranger-approaching growls sound quite similar (at least, to our ears), they prompted different behaviour.

Many studies investigating vocalisations are based on pre-recorded samples, but it’s important to remember that vocalisations and visual signals usually go hand-in-hand. In the stranger-approaching context, dogs growled with closed mouths, whereas in food defence situations, they showed their teeth and pulled back their lips.

Barks

While we tend to take notice when we hear a growl, we often dismiss barking as meaningless noise, as though it’s simply an item on a dog’s daily checklist: take a walk, have breakfast, bark. Before the turn of the century, that was the prevailing view among researchers and theorists. At most, barking was thought to result from social facilitation – one dog barking prompts other dogs to bark – or maybe attention seeking, or even rivalry or defence.

Only recently have researchers begun to investigate whether barks produced in different contexts vary in their acoustic parameters (such as tone and pitch). Scientists theorised that if – like growls – barks displayed consistent differences, they might have a more specific communicative function, perhaps even be associated with a dog’s internal motivational or emotional state. For example, some barks might convey aggression, while others might convey friendliness.

In one early study Dr Sophia Yin, DVM, recorded a variety of breeds barking in response to different situations: a stranger ringing the doorbell (‘disturbance barks’), separated from an owner (‘isolation barks’) and play. Dr Yin found that the barks did indeed have different acoustic properties. Disturbance barks were harsher and lower in pitch with little amplitude modulation, while isolation and play barks were pitched higher and had greater tonal and higher frequency, and a wider range of amplitude modulation. More recent studies confirm that dog barks follow particular patterns. For example, a dog barking at a stranger sounds very different from a dog barking before going on a walk.

But do these vocalisations carry meaning? They do for dogs. When dogs in one study listened to barks recorded in a new context or from a new dog, they gave more attention to the unfamiliar bark. This suggests that dogs can detect that some barks are different from others, though scientists are still exploring ways to determine how exactly they perceive and process that information.

Humans, too, can decipher barks. Whether or not they’re experienced with dogs, people are quite good at classifying barks into their appropriate contexts and attributing them to perceived emotional states. After listening to randomly played recordings, people describe isolation barks as full of despair, while barks from a play session are said to be happy. (So you’re probably spot-on that your neighbour’s Dalmatian who yaps all day is lonely.) Our ability to do this starts early; by age 10, children are able to assign different-sounding barks to the correct context. Today, we can distinguish the acoustic properties of certain barks so accurately that we’re able to program computers to categorise them.

Recognising the audible patterns

Apparently, when it comes to chatting, dogs and humans have more in common than grumbling when it’s dinnertime. Through their shared mammalian histories, canine and human vocalisations follow similar acoustic patterns. High-pitched and more tonal noises convey friendliness, affiliation and ‘come here’, whereas low-pitched and less tonal sounds convey aggression and ‘go away’. These rules and tendencies, which are found across mammalian and avian species, govern our own communication and emotional expression.

Internal motivational state of conflict

A recent publication by Kathryn Lord, a Research Specialist at UMass Medical School, offers an additional take on why dogs bark. She and her collaborator, the late Raymond Coppinger, investigated the contexts in which other species use bark-like sounds: “When other species emit their version of a bark, they are usually in some sort of conflicting situation. For example, an animal is at a nest or den and observes some sort of threat. Customarily, the animal would run, but because of their situation, they can’t, so they bark,” she explains. “We think that when dogs bark, they are making these sounds in association with an alert or an internal motivational state of conflict.”

More domesticated situations = more barks

Lord and Coppinger’s research suggests that dogs bark in so many different situations because they often find themselves conflicted – they are in the house and want to go out, they are out and want to come in. And it may be that, through the process of domestication itself, dogs have become more prone to putting themselves in these sorts of situations. In comparison with wolves, dogs have a substantially decreased flight distance; something can easily get too close before the dog feels conflicted about how to respond.

Udell suggests that barking doesn’t have to be whittled down to one simple explanation. “If you look at communication and vocalisations in a wide range of species, it usually isn’t about one thing,” he says. “Chickadees have ‘alert’ calls, but they also have songs, and the songs themselves can mean different things in different contexts. I think the same could hold true for dogs.”

Other reasons for non-stop barking

These general insights are just part of the story. Genes and environment affect all things dog, including vocalisations. In their seminal study of dog behaviour and genetics, John Paul Scott and John Fuller note that when Basenjis, a typically ‘barkless’ dog, do actually bark, they generally produce only one or two low ‘woofs’. On the other hand, “The maximum number of barks recorded for a Cocker Spaniel in a 10-minute period was 907, or more than 90 a minute.” (Why Guinness World Records was not contacted is beyond us.)

But genes aren’t everything. “While Shih Tzus as a group tend to display less barking than Miniature Poodles, that doesn’t mean barking in Miniature Poodles is impervious to change,” explains Susan Friedman, a pioneer in the application of applied behaviour analysis to captive and companion animals, and a psychology professor at Utah State University. “And I’ve certainly known individual Miniature Poodles who are quiet and individual Shih Tzus who are barky, both based on their current situations. The individual always bests any generalisation.”

Dr Yin’s study about dog barks supports this theory. Even within breeds, she found variations in who barked and when. Rudy and Siggy, 11-year-old German Shorthaired Pointers, both barked in the disturbance context, but when alone, Rudy did not bark and Siggy had lots to say.

And then there are the effects of a pup’s social environment on their behaviour: for example, feral dogs are much less noisy than their counterparts who play with toys, sleep in beds and go to obedience class. Friedman explains: “For dogs, barking is a functional behaviour, meaning it’s maintained, increased or decreased due to consequences. Once this is understood, it opens the door to changing the duration, intensity and frequency of the behaviour by changing the consequences.” In other words, dogs can learn to be quieter.

However, perfect quiet is probably unrealistic. Pet parents can’t always control the stimuli that prompt barking, especially if they’re not home 24-7. Moreover, barking that has been solidified and maintained over time through intermittent reinforcement has a lot of staying power. “It seems that owners unintentionally reinforce the barks produced when a dog is around food or toys, and these become the begging barks of that dog,” says Faragó. 

Laura Monaco Torelli, director of training at Animal Behavior Training Concepts, agrees. “If a dog learns that the noise in the hallway goes away when he barks, barking becomes an effective behaviour. Barking is followed by the consequence of the noise in the hallway stopping.”

Strategies for change

So what can dog parents do about barking? Before you get carried away, consider whether or not action is even required. Friedman advises taking a step back. “When we ask, ‘Is barking a behaviour problem?’ The next question is, ‘For whom?’ Barking certainly is a problem when people say it is, and for dogs, it is a problem when they are spending so much time doing it that it eclipses other healthful activities.”

Pet parents should focus not on eliminating barking altogether, but on reducing it to levels they find appropriate and liveable. When she meets with clients to discuss their dogs’ barking issues, Monaco Torelli asks questions such as, ‘How many barks is OK? What’s excessive to you?’ This, she says, gives the trainer a good starting point from which to develop a plan to teach the client how to reshape a dog’s barking behaviour. Trainers and pet parents discuss acceptable barking, and then implement techniques to achieve desired levels in each context.

Friedman shares the way she manages her own dog’s barking: “We live in the country, and when we let the dogs out, they bark at the deer for a number of seconds. Then we say, ‘That’s enough, thank you,’ and they are quiet and we praise them.” She adds, “It’s a mistake to think that barking is the problem. The real problem is that dogs don’t stop barking when we ask.”

So-called ‘quick fixes’ can make barking worse, particularly if the underlying reason for the behaviour isn’t addressed. “Putting an anti-bark collar on a fearful dog is unlikely to decrease barking if the consequence [shock or spray] increases the dog’s fear. If the fear increases, barking could as well,” explains animal behaviourist Mary Huntsberry. 

It helps to start early and be observant – teaching dogs the boundaries of acceptable vocalisations from an early age will pay off for everyone. When pups are young, barking might be cute, but as they age, the cute factor tends to wear off. If the behaviour is already in place, there are ways to alter it, Huntsberry observes. “It helps to do a functional analysis. During an extensive interview, I identify what happens immediately before and after the unwanted behaviour so I can identify the trigger and what maintains it.”

Monaco Torelli focuses her attention on the dog-human relationship. “When owners are frustrated by their dog’s behaviour, we show them some immediate training goals and success points so they see that their dog can do what they want them to be doing, instead of what they don’t want them to do,” she says. This helps them rebuild their bond with their dog. 

The takeaway message is that barking is a nuanced and flexible behaviour, and relationships can grow by paying attention to what your dog’s vocalisations mean. Maybe all that whining for a walk means your pup knows you both need to step away from social media for a second and get outside.

Julie Hecht

Julie Hecht, MSc

Julie Hecht, MSc, is a PhD candidate studying animal behavior at the Graduate Center, CUNY, and author of the Dog Spies blog at Scientific American. She would really like to meet your dog.

Related articles